EXPLORING AMERICANS' ATTITUDES TOWARD SOCIALIZERS AND CHILDREARING RECIPES

AMERICANS' PERCEPTIONS OF THE RELATIVE
IMPACTS OF DIFFERENT SOCIALIZERS

In the "biographical autopsies" the mass media conducts following the death of some infamous individual (i.e., Rev. Jimmy Jones following the mass suicide of his religious cult in the jungles of Guyana, or such serial killers as Jeffrey Dahmer), frequently revealed is a story of abuse and inability or (or lack of interest) of parents intervening to alter the self development of their child. But not all victims of abuse grow up to become deviants. What often makes the difference is having some supportive adult.

In the 1990 NORC General Survey, a random sample of American adults were asked the following series of questions: "Many adults play roles in the lives of children. Please rate the following as very good (coded 1), good (2), fairly good (3), fairly bad (4), and very bad (5): mothers, fathers, grandparents, teachers, and priests, ministers or rabbis." Below are the mean scores white and black females and males gave (note: the lower the mean the more highly rated the individual's role).

MEAN SCORES GIVEN TO ADULTS' ROLES IN THE LIVES OF CHILDREN
BY WHITE AND BLACK FEMALES AND MALES

WH FEM BL FEM BL MAL WH MAL TOTAL
MOM 1.61 1.54 1.50 1.58 1.59
GPAR 1.72 1.57 1.49 1.77 1.72
DAD 1.84 2.00 1.77 1.81 1.84
CLERGY 2.05 1.90 1.87 2.15 2.07
TEACHER 2.24 2.17 1.97 2.28 2.24

Do you detect any interesting differences here? First, there are statistically significant differences in the mean scores given by black and white males and females to the roles played by grandparents, clergy, and teachers. Second, it is interesting how grandparents are given the edge over fathers, particularly among African-Americans.



H ISTORICAL CHANGES IN CHILDREARING STRATEGIES

Spare the rod and spoil the child.

There have been intriguing shifts over the past century in popularized strategies for creating "good" selves. Part of this change involves shifts in connotations of "good," whether it means something good from the perspective of society or of the individual being socialized. As Warren Susman (Culture as History) argues, between the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, there has been a cultural shift in the predominant self-structure of Americans, from the romantic ideal of "character" to one of "personality." Character involves matters of integrity and duty; personality involves popularity, the ability to make friends. Associated with these changes are parenting strategies ranging on at least one continuum from "permissiveness" to authoritarianism.

This longitudinal shift toward permissiveness is clearly evident in Wolfenstein's research on infant genital exploration, where he analyzed Infant Care, a government-issued child care pamphlet. In the 1914 version, physical restraints were advised for children in bed, with warnings that masturbation was an injurious practice. Fifteen years later, masturbation was seen as a short, passing phase, and experts recommended giving the child a toy to hold at bedtime. By 1951, masturbation was seen as an act about which the child had no feelings and was only a source of maternal embarrassment. In their four-generation model of American history, Strauss & Howe (1991:65) see more cyclical trends, arguing that shifts in child nurture typically occur during or just after eras of exceptional spiritual fervor (for example, the 1741-2, 1842-3, 1981-2) or just following major crises in public life (the 1859-60 or 1942-43).


IT IS IMPORTANT THAT THE CHILD MUST OBEY

The thing that impresses me most about America is the way parents obey their children.
--Duke of Windsor

Among the differences between the social classes is the way they socialize their young. Elsewhere, for instance, we considered class differences in endorsing spanking. The results of a 1989 national survey of Americans revealed that individuals identifying themselves as members of the lower class to be seven times more likely than those identifying themselves as members of the upper class (and nearly twice as likely as those from the middle and working classes) to mention obedience as "the most important [thing] for a child to learn," and less than half as likely to mention thinking for oneself. The explanation for this is that in order to survive, lower-class individuals must know how to obey and to defer to others.

Over the years, the NORC General Social Surveys have included questions dealing with socialization strategies for children (the 1989 study above, in fact, was one these survey years). Among the questions asked were the following two dealing with the importance placed on obedience: